https://www.mdu.se/

mdu.sePublications
Change search
Refine search result
123 1 - 50 of 149
CiteExportLink to result list
Permanent link
Cite
Citation style
  • apa
  • ieee
  • modern-language-association-8th-edition
  • vancouver
  • Other style
More styles
Language
  • de-DE
  • en-GB
  • en-US
  • fi-FI
  • nn-NO
  • nn-NB
  • sv-SE
  • Other locale
More languages
Output format
  • html
  • text
  • asciidoc
  • rtf
Rows per page
  • 5
  • 10
  • 20
  • 50
  • 100
  • 250
Sort
  • Standard (Relevance)
  • Author A-Ö
  • Author Ö-A
  • Title A-Ö
  • Title Ö-A
  • Publication type A-Ö
  • Publication type Ö-A
  • Issued (Oldest first)
  • Issued (Newest first)
  • Created (Oldest first)
  • Created (Newest first)
  • Last updated (Oldest first)
  • Last updated (Newest first)
  • Disputation date (earliest first)
  • Disputation date (latest first)
  • Standard (Relevance)
  • Author A-Ö
  • Author Ö-A
  • Title A-Ö
  • Title Ö-A
  • Publication type A-Ö
  • Publication type Ö-A
  • Issued (Oldest first)
  • Issued (Newest first)
  • Created (Oldest first)
  • Created (Newest first)
  • Last updated (Oldest first)
  • Last updated (Newest first)
  • Disputation date (earliest first)
  • Disputation date (latest first)
Select
The maximal number of hits you can export is 250. When you want to export more records please use the Create feeds function.
  • 1. Andersson, P. A.
    et al.
    Vartanova, I.
    Västfjäll, D.
    Tinghög, G.
    Strimling, P.
    Wu, J.
    Hazin, I.
    Akotia, C. S.
    Aldashev, A.
    Andrighetto, Giulia
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Anum, A.
    Arikan, G.
    Bagherian, F.
    Barrera, D.
    Basnight-Brown, D.
    Batkeyev, B.
    Berezina, E.
    Björnstjerna, M.
    Boski, P.
    Bovina, I.
    Huyen, B. T. T.
    Čekrlija, Đ.
    Choi, H. -S
    Contreras-Ibáñez, C. C.
    Costa-Lopes, R.
    de Barra, M.
    de Zoysa, P.
    Dorrough, A. R.
    Dvoryanchikov, N.
    Engelmann, J. B.
    Euh, H.
    Fang, X.
    Fiedler, S.
    Foster-Gimbel, O. A.
    Fülöp, M.
    Gardarsdottir, R. B.
    Gill, C. M. H. D.
    Glöckner, A.
    Graf, S.
    Grigoryan, A.
    Gritskov, V.
    Growiec, K.
    Halama, P.
    Hartanto, A.
    Hopthrow, T.
    Hřebíčková, M.
    Iliško, D.
    Imada, H.
    Kapoor, H.
    Kawakami, K.
    Khachatryan, N.
    Kharchenko, N.
    Kiyonari, T.
    Kohút, M.
    Leslie, L. M.
    Li, Y.
    Li, N. P.
    Li, Z.
    Liik, K.
    Maitner, A. T.
    Manhique, B.
    Manley, H.
    Medhioub, I.
    Mentser, S.
    Nejat, P.
    Nipassa, O.
    Nussinson, R.
    Onyedire, N. G.
    Onyishi, I. E.
    Panagiotopoulou, P.
    Perez-Floriano, L. R.
    Persson, M.
    Pirttilä-Backman, A. -M
    Pogosyan, M.
    Raver, J.
    Rodrigues, R. B.
    Romanò, S.
    Romero, P. P.
    Sakki, I.
    San Martin, A.
    Sherbaji, S.
    Shimizu, H.
    Simpson, B.
    Szabo, E.
    Takemura, K.
    Teixeira, M. L. M.
    Thanomkul, N.
    Tiliouine, H.
    Travaglino, G. A.
    Tsirbas, Y.
    Widodo, S.
    Zein, R.
    Zirganou-Kazolea, L.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Anger and disgust shape judgments of social sanctions across cultures, especially in high individual autonomy societies2024In: Scientific Reports, E-ISSN 2045-2322, Vol. 14, no 1, article id 5591Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    When someone violates a social norm, others may think that some sanction would be appropriate. We examine how the experience of emotions like anger and disgust relate to the judged appropriateness of sanctions, in a pre-registered analysis of data from a large-scale study in 56 societies. Across the world, we find that individuals who experience anger and disgust over a norm violation are more likely to endorse confrontation, ostracism and, to a smaller extent, gossip. Moreover, we find that the experience of anger is consistently the strongest predictor of judgments of confrontation, compared to other emotions. Although the link between state-based emotions and judgments may seem universal, its strength varies across countries. Aligned with theoretical predictions, this link is stronger in societies, and among individuals, that place higher value on individual autonomy. Thus, autonomy values may increase the role that emotions play in guiding judgments of social sanctions.

  • 2.
    Andrews, George
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Integer Partitions2004Book (Other (popular science, discussion, etc.))
  • 3.
    Andrighetto, Giulia
    et al.
    Natl Res Council Italy, Inst Cognit Sci & Technol, Rome, Italy.;Inst Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.;Linköping Univ, Inst Analyt Sociol, Linköping, Sweden..
    Szekely, Aron
    Natl Res Council Italy, Inst Cognit Sci & Technol, Rome, Italy.;Coll Carlo Alberto, Turin, Italy..
    Guido, Andrea
    Natl Res Council Italy, Inst Cognit Sci & Technol, Rome, Italy.;Inst Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.;Univ Bourgogne Franche Comte, Burgundy Sch Business, CEREN EA 7477, Dijon, France..
    Gelfand, Michele
    Stanford Univ, Grad Sch Business, Stanford, CA USA.;Stanford Univ, Dept Psychol, Stanford, CA USA..
    Abemathy, Jered
    Univ South Carolina, Dept Sociol, Columbia, SC USA..
    Arikan, Gizem
    Trinity Coll Dublin, Dept Polit Sci, Dublin, Ireland..
    Aycan, Zeynep
    Koc Univ, Dept Psychol, Istanbul, Turkiye.;Koc Univ, Fac Management, Istanbul, Turkiye..
    Bankar, Shweta
    Ashoka Univ, Sonipat, India..
    Barrera, Davide
    Coll Carlo Alberto, Turin, Italy.;Univ Turin, Dept Culture Polit & Soc, Turin, Italy..
    Basnight-Brown, Dana
    US Int Univ Africa, Nairobi, Kenya..
    Belaus, Anabel
    Consejo Nacl Invest Cient & Tecn CONICET, Inst Invest Psicol IIPsi, Cordoba, Argentina.;Univ Nacl Cordoba, Fac Psicol, Cordoba, Argentina..
    Berezina, Elizaveta
    Sunway Univ, Bandar Sunway, Malaysia..
    Blumen, Sheyla
    Pontificia Univ Catolica Peru, Dept Psicol, Lima, Peru..
    Boski, Pawel
    SWPS Univ, Warsaw, Poland..
    Bui, Huyen Thi Thu
    Camilo Cardenas, Juan
    Univ Los Andes, Bogota, Colombia.;Univ Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA USA..
    Cekrlija, Dorde
    Univ Banja Luka, Fac Philosophy, Banja Luka, Bosnia & Herceg.;Univ Greifswald, Inst Psychol, Greifswald, Germany..
    de Barra, Micheal
    Brunel Univ London, Ctr Culture & Evolut, Uxbridge, Middx, England..
    de Zoysa, Piyanjali
    Univ Colombo, Fac Med, Colombo, Sri Lanka..
    Dorrough, Angela
    Univ Cologne, Dept Psychol, Cologne, Germany..
    Engelmann, Jan B.
    Univ Amsterdam, Amsterdam Sch Econ, Ctr Res Expt Econ & Polit Decis Making CREED, Amsterdam, Netherlands..
    Euh, Hyun
    Univ Illinois, Gies Coll Business, Champaign, IL USA..
    Fiedler, Susann
    Vienna Univ Econ & Business, Vienna, Austria..
    Foster-Gimbel, Olivia
    NYU, Sch Business, New York, NY USA..
    Freitas, Goncalo
    Univ Lisbon, Inst Ciencias Sociais, Lisbon, Portugal..
    Fulop, Marta
    HUN REN Inst Cognit Neurosci & Psychol, Res Ctr Nat Sci, Budapest, Hungary.;Karoli Gaspar Univ Reformed Churches, Inst Psychol, Budapest, Hungary..
    Gardarsdottir, Ragna B.
    Univ Iceland, Fac Psychol, Reykjavik, Iceland..
    Gill, Colin Mathew Hugues D.
    Sunway Univ, Bandar Sunway, Malaysia.;Universal Coll Bangladesh, Dhaka, Bangladesh..
    Gloeckner, Andreas
    Univ Cologne, Dept Psychol, Cologne, Germany..
    Graf, Sylvie
    Czech Acad Sci, Inst Psychol, Brno, Czech Republic..
    Grigoryan, Ani
    Yerevan State Univ, Dept Personal Psychol, Yerevan, Armenia..
    Growiec, Katarzyna
    SWPS Univ, Warsaw, Poland..
    Hashimoto, Hirofumi
    Osaka Metropolitan Univ, Osaka, Japan..
    Hopthrow, Tim
    Univ Kent, Sch Psychol, Canterbury, Kent, England..
    Hrebickova, Martina
    Czech Acad Sci, Inst Psychol, Brno, Czech Republic..
    Imada, Hirotaka
    Royal Holloway Univ London, Egham, Surrey, England..
    Kamijo, Yoshio
    Waseda Univ, Tokyo, Japan..
    Kapoor, Hansika
    Monk Prayogshala, Dept Psychol, Mumbai, Maharashtra, India..
    Kashima, Yoshihisa
    Univ Melbourne, Melbourne Sch Psychol Sci, Melbourne, Vic, Australia..
    Khachatryan, Narine
    Yerevan State Univ, Dept Personal Psychol, Yerevan, Armenia..
    Kharchenko, Natalia
    Kyiv Int Inst Sociol, Kiev, Ukraine..
    Leon, Diana
    DeJusticia, Bogota, Colombia..
    Leslie, Lisa M.
    NYU, Sch Business, New York, NY USA..
    Li, Yang
    Nagoya Univ, Nagoya, Aichi, Japan..
    Liik, Kadi
    Tallinn Univ, Sch Nat Sci & Hlth, Tallinn, Estonia..
    Liuzza, Marco Tullio
    Magna Graecia Univ Catanzaro, Dept Med & Surg Sci, Catanzaro, Italy..
    Maitner, Angela T.
    Amer Univ Sharjah, Dept Psychol, Sharjah, U Arab Emirates..
    Mamidi, Pavan
    Ashoka Univ, Sonipat, India..
    McArdle, Michele
    Trinity Coll Dublin, Dept Polit Sci, Dublin, Ireland..
    Medhioub, Imed
    Imam Mohammad Ibn Saud Islamic Univ, Dept Finance & Investment, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia..
    Mendes Teixeira, Maria Luisa
    Presbyterian Mackenzie Univ, Sao Paulo, Brazil..
    Mentser, Sari
    Hebrew Univ Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel..
    Morales, Francisco
    Univ Andes, Santiago, Chile..
    Narayanan, Jayanth
    Northeastern Univ, Boston, MA USA..
    Nitta, Kohei
    Ritsumeikan Univ, Shiga, Japan..
    Nussinson, Ravit
    Open Univ Israel, Dept Educ & Psychol, Raanana, Israel.;Univ Haifa, IIPDM, Haifa, Israel..
    Onyedire, Nneoma G.
    Univ Nigeria, Dept Psychol, Nsukka, Nigeria..
    Onyishi, Ike E.
    Univ Nigeria, Dept Psychol, Nsukka, Nigeria..
    Osin, Evgeny
    HSE Univ, Moscow, Russia..
    Ozden, Seniha
    Koc Univ, Dept Psychol, Istanbul, Turkiye..
    Panagiotopoulou, Penny
    Univ Patras, Dept Educ & Social Work, Patras, Greece..
    Pereverziev, Oleksandr
    POLLSTER, Kiev, Ukraine..
    Perez-Floriano, Lorena R.
    Univ Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile..
    Pirttila-Backman, Anna-Maija
    Univ Helsinki, Fac Social Sci, Social Psychol, Helsinki, Finland..
    Pogosyan, Marianna
    Univ Amsterdam, Amsterdam Business Sch ABS, Leadership & Management, Amsterdam, Netherlands..
    Raver, Jana
    Queens Univ, Kingston, ON, Canada..
    Reyna, Cecilia
    Consejo Nacl Invest Cient & Tecn CONICET, Inst Invest Psicol IIPsi, Cordoba, Argentina..
    Rodrigues, Ricardo Borges
    Inst Univ Lisboa ISCTE IUL, Ctr Invest & Intervencao Social, Lisbon, Portugal..
    Romano, Sara
    Univ Turin, Dept Culture Polit & Soc, Turin, Italy..
    Romero, Pedro P.
    Univ San Francisco Quito, Sch Econ, Quito, Ecuador.;Univ San Francisco Quito, ECEL, Quito, Ecuador..
    Sakki, Inari
    Univ Helsinki, Fac Social Sci, Social Psychol, Helsinki, Finland..
    Sanchez, Angel
    Univ Carlos III Madrid, Dept Matemat, GISC, Leganes, Spain.;Univ Zaragoza, Inst Biocomputac & Fis Sistemas Complejos BIFI, Zaragoza, Spain..
    Sherbaji, Sara
    Amer Univ Sharjah, Dept Psychol, Sharjah, U Arab Emirates.;UCL, Dept Anthropol, London, England..
    Simpson, Brent
    Univ South Carolina, Dept Sociol, Columbia, SC USA..
    Spadoni, Lorenzo
    Univ Cassino & Southern Lazio, Dept Econ & Law, Cassino, FR, Italy..
    Stamkou, Eftychia
    Univ Amsterdam, Dept Psychol, Amsterdam, Netherlands..
    Travaglino, Giovanni A.
    Royal Holloway Univ London, Egham, Surrey, England..
    Van Lange, Paul A. M.
    Vrije Univ, Dept Expt & Appl Psychol, Amsterdam, Netherlands..
    Winata, Fiona Fira
    Univ Airlangga, Fac Psychol, Surabaya, Indonesia..
    Zein, Rizqy Amelia
    Univ Airlangga, Fac Psychol, Surabaya, Indonesia..
    Zhang, Qing-peng
    Guangzhou Univ, Guangzhou, Peoples R China..
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Inst Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.;Stockholm Univ, Ctr Cultural Evolut, Stockholm.
    Changes in social norms during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic across 43 countries2024In: Nature Communications, E-ISSN 2041-1723, Vol. 15, no 1, article id 1436Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    The emergence of COVID-19 dramatically changed social behavior across societies and contexts. Here we study whether social norms also changed. Specifically, we study this question for cultural tightness (the degree to which societies generally have strong norms), specific social norms (e.g. stealing, hand washing), and norms about enforcement, using survey data from 30,431 respondents in 43 countries recorded before and in the early stages following the emergence of COVID-19. Using variation in disease intensity, we shed light on the mechanisms predicting changes in social norm measures. We find evidence that, after the emergence of the COVID-19 pandemic, hand washing norms increased while tightness and punishing frequency slightly decreased but observe no evidence for a robust change in most other norms. Thus, at least in the short term, our findings suggest that cultures are largely stable to pandemic threats except in those norms, hand washing in this case, that are perceived to be directly relevant to dealing with the collective threat. Tightness-looseness theory predicts that social norms strengthen following threat. Here the authors test this and find that, after the emergence of the COVID-19 pandemic, hand washing norms increased, but no evidence was observed for a robust change in most other norms.

  • 4.
    Bernard, Mark
    et al.
    Maastricht University .
    Dreber, Anna
    Stockholm School of Economics.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Stockholm University.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    The subgroup problem: When can binding voting on extractions from a common resource pool overcome the tragedy of the commons?2013In: Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, ISSN 0167-2681, E-ISSN 1879-1751, Vol. 91, p. 122-130Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Using a common pool resource game protocol with voting we examine experimentally how cooperation varies with the level at which (binding) votes are aggregated. Our results are broadly in line with theoretical predictions. When players can vote on the behavior of the whole group or when leaders from each group can vote for the group as a whole, extraction levels from the common resource pool are close to the social optimum. When players extract resources individually, there is substantial overextraction. When players vote in subgroups, there is initially less overextraction but it increases over time. This suggests that in order for binding voting to overcome the tragedy of the commons in social dilemmas, it should ideally affect the group as a whole.

  • 5.
    Cownden, Daniel
    et al.
    Univ Glasgow, Scotland.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm Univ, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Stockholm Univ, Stockholm, Sweden.
    A popular misapplication of evolutionary modeling to the study of human cooperation2017In: Evolution and human behavior, ISSN 1090-5138, E-ISSN 1879-0607, Vol. 38, no 3, p. 421-427Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    To examine the evolutionary basis of a behavior, an established approach (known as the phenotypic gambit) is to assume that the behavior is controlled by a single allele, the fitness effects of which are derived from a consideration of how the behavior interacts, via life-history, with other ecological factors. Here we contrast successful applications of this approach with several examples of an influential and superficially similar line of research on the evolutionary basis of human cooperation. A key difference is identified: in the latter line of research the focal behavior, cooperation, is abstractly defined in terms of immediate fitness costs and benefits. Selection is then assumed to act on strategies in an iterated social context for which fitness effects can be derived by aggregation of the abstractly defined immediate fitness effects over a lifetime. This approach creates a closed theoretical loop, rendering models incapable of making predictions or providing insight into the origin of human cooperation. We conclude with a discussion of how evolutionary approaches might be appropriately used in the study of human social behavior. (C) 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 6.
    Cownden, Daniel
    et al.
    University of St Andrews, Scotland.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm University, Sweden.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Stockholm University, Sweden.
    The implications of learning across perceptually and strategically distinct situations2018In: Synthese, ISSN 0039-7857, E-ISSN 1573-0964, Vol. 195, no 2, p. 511-528Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Game theory is a formal approach to behavior that focuses on the strategic aspect of situations. The game theoretic approach originates in economics but has been embraced by scholars across disciplines, including many philosophers and biologists. This approach has an important weakness: the strategic aspect of a situation, which is its defining quality in game theory, is often not its most salient quality in human (or animal) cognition. Evidence from a wide range of experiments highlights this shortcoming. Previous theoretical and empirical work has sought to address this weakness by considering learning across an ensemble of multiple games simultaneously. Here we extend this framework, incorporating artificial neural networks, to allow for an investigation of the interaction between the perceptual and functional similarity of the games composing the larger ensemble. Using this framework, we conduct a theoretical investigation of a population that encounters both stag hunts and prisoner's dilemmas, two situations that are strategically different but which may or may not be perceptually similar.

    Download full text (pdf)
    Cownden_Eriksson_Strimling
  • 7.
    de Barra, Mícheál
    et al.
    Centre for the Study of Cultural Evolution, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Centre for the Study of Cultural Evolution, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Centre for the Study of Cultural Evolution, Stockholm, Sweden.
    How feedback biases give ineffective medical treatments a good reputation.2014In: Journal of medical Internet research, ISSN 1438-8871, Vol. 16, no 8, p. e193-Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    BACKGROUND: Medical treatments with no direct effect (like homeopathy) or that cause harm (like bloodletting) are common across cultures and throughout history. How do such treatments spread and persist? Most medical treatments result in a range of outcomes: some people improve while others deteriorate. If the people who improve are more inclined to tell others about their experiences than the people who deteriorate, ineffective or even harmful treatments can maintain a good reputation.

    OBJECTIVE: The intent of this study was to test the hypothesis that positive outcomes are overrepresented in online medical product reviews, to examine if this reputational distortion is large enough to bias people's decisions, and to explore the implications of this bias for the cultural evolution of medical treatments.

    METHODS: We compared outcomes of weight loss treatments and fertility treatments in clinical trials to outcomes reported in 1901 reviews on Amazon. Then, in a series of experiments, we evaluated people's choice of weight loss diet after reading different reviews. Finally, a mathematical model was used to examine if this bias could result in less effective treatments having a better reputation than more effective treatments.

    RESULTS: Data are consistent with the hypothesis that people with better outcomes are more inclined to write reviews. After 6 months on the diet, 93% (64/69) of online reviewers reported a weight loss of 10 kg or more while just 27% (19/71) of clinical trial participants experienced this level of weight change. A similar positive distortion was found in fertility treatment reviews. In a series of experiments, we show that people are more inclined to begin a diet with many positive reviews, than a diet with reviews that are representative of the diet's true effect. A mathematical model of medical cultural evolution shows that the size of the positive distortion critically depends on the shape of the outcome distribution.

    CONCLUSIONS: Online reviews overestimate the benefits of medical treatments, probably because people with negative outcomes are less inclined to tell others about their experiences. This bias can enable ineffective medical treatments to maintain a good reputation.

  • 8.
    Dorrough, Angela Rachael
    et al.
    Univ Cologne, Social Cognit Ctr, Cologne, Germany..
    Froehlich, Laura
    Fernuniv, Res Cluster D2L2, Hagen, Germany..
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Inst Futures Studies, Stockholm.
    Cooperation in the cross-national context2022In: CURRENT OPINION IN PSYCHOLOGY, ISSN 2352-250X, Vol. 44, p. 281-285Article, review/survey (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    In this article, we outline the current state of research concerning cooperation in the cross-national context. We present several theoretical approaches and empirical findings regarding national differences in cooperation, as well as how cooperation may depend on the national background of the interaction partner. In addition, we discuss the influence of (national) group norms, cultural similarity, and ingroup membership. This review concludes with a call for research on cooperation to include more non-WEIRD nations and more systematically cover national background as one important social category determining the willingness to cooperate.

  • 9.
    Engström, E.
    et al.
    Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Björnstjerna, M.
    Lund University, Lund, Sweden.
    Strimling, P.
    Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Global variations in online privacy concerns across 57 countries2023In: Computers in Human Behavior Reports, ISSN 2451-9588, Vol. 9, article id 100268Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Cross-cultural studies have found national differences in how concerned people are about online privacy. However, it has not yet been settled what causes this variation, and several factors have been proposed in the literature, including internet habituation, individualism, and uncertainty avoidance. Here we investigate these factors by two studies. In the first, we examine the association between online privacy concerns and a new measure of online self-disclosure norms that we introduce. We find that this measure is significantly associated with two established instruments of online privacy concerns in the literature. In the second, we analyze previously unpublished data from a questionnaire on online self-disclosure norms as assessed by this new measure. It includes replies from 18,046 adult respondents from 57 countries and six continents. We find that norms in favor of more restrictive online self-disclosure are weaker in countries with higher levels of internet penetration (r = −0.56, p < .001). Our findings suggest that higher internet penetration in a country reduces online privacy concerns. The results support the idea that habituation to online environments decreases privacy risk perceptions. An implication is that preferences for online privacy are likely to decline over time in countries where internet penetration is still low. Lastly, in conflict with previous studies, our analyses do not support the theory that online privacy concerns are associated with national cultures related to individualism or uncertainty avoidance as measured by Hofstede's indices. 

  • 10. Enquist, Magnus
    et al.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Mänsklig kultur – vad är det för speciellt med den?2007In: Tvärsnitt, no 2, p. 10-11Article in journal (Other (popular science, discussion, etc.))
  • 11.
    Enquist, Magnus
    et al.
    Stockholm University.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Ghirlanda, Stefano
    Stockholm University.
    Critical Social Learning: A Solution to Rogers's Paradox of Nonadaptive Culture2007In: American Anthropologist, ISSN 0002-7294, E-ISSN 1548-1433, Vol. 109, no 4, p. 727-734Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Alan Rogers (1988) presented a game theory model of the evolution of social learning, yielding the paradoxical conclusion that social learning does not increase the fitness of a population. We expand on this model, allowing for imperfections in individual and social learning as well as incorporating a "critical social learning" strategy that tries to solve an adaptive problem first by social learning, and then by individual learning if socially acquired behavior proves unsatisfactory. This strategy always proves superior to pure social learning and typically has higher fitness than pure individual learning, providing a solution to Rogers's paradox of nonadaptive culture. Critical social learning is an evolutionarily stable strategy (ESS) unless cultural transmission is highly unfaithful, the environment is highly variable, or social learning is much more costly than individual learning. We compare the model to empirical data on social learning and on spatial variation in primate cultures and list three requirements for adaptive culture. © 2007 by the American Anthropological Association.

  • 12.
    Enquist, Magnus
    et al.
    Stockholms universitet, Centrum för evolutionär kulturforskning.
    Ghirlanda, Stefano
    Brooklyn College of the City University New York, Department of Psychology and Honors Academy.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Modelling the evolution and diversity of cumulative culture2011In: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences, ISSN 0962-8436, E-ISSN 1471-2970, Vol. 366, no 1563, p. 412-423Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Previous work on mathematical models of cultural evolution has mainly focused on the diffusion of simple cultural elements. However, a characteristic feature of human cultural evolution is the seemingly limitless appearance of new and increasingly complex cultural elements. Here, we develop a general modelling framework to study such cumulative processes, in which we assume that the appearance and disappearance of cultural elements are stochastic events that depend on the current state of culture. Five scenarios are explored: evolution of independent cultural elements, stepwise modification of elements, differentiation or combination of elements and systems of cultural elements. As one application of our framework, we study the evolution of cultural diversity (in time as well as between groups).

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 13.
    Enquist, Magnus
    et al.
    Stockholm University.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Stockholm University.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Laland, Kevin
    University of St Andrews.
    Sjöstrand, Jonas
    Stockholm University.
    One cultural parent makes no culture2010In: Animal Behaviour, ISSN 0003-3472, E-ISSN 1095-8282, Vol. 79, no 6, p. 1353-1362Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    The ability to acquire knowledge and skills from others is widespread in animals and is commonly thought to be responsible for the behavioural traditions observed in many species. However, in spite of the extensive literature on theoretical analyses and empirical studies of social learning, little attention has been given to whether individuals acquire knowledge from a single individual or multiple models. Researchers commonly refer to instances of sons learning from fathers, or daughters from mothers, while theoreticians have constructed models of uniparental transmission, with little consideration of whether such restricted modes of transmission are actually feasible. We used mathematical models to demonstrate that the conditions under which learning from a single cultural parent can lead to stable culture are surprisingly restricted ( the same reasoning applies to a single social-learning event). Conversely, we demonstrate how learning from more than one cultural parent can establish culture, and find that cultural traits will reach a nonzero equilibrium in the population provided the product of the fidelity of social learning and the number of cultural parents exceeds 1. We discuss the implications of the analysis for interpreting various findings in the animal social-learning literature, as well as the unique features of human culture.

  • 14. Eriksson, Henrik
    et al.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Conjugacy of Coxeter elements2009In: The Electronic Journal of Combinatorics, ISSN 1097-1440, E-ISSN 1077-8926, Vol. 16, no 2, p. R4-Article in journal (Refereed)
  • 15. Eriksson, Henrik
    et al.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Words with intervening neighbours in infinite Coxeter groups are reduced2010In: The Electronic Journal of Combinatorics, ISSN 1097-1440, E-ISSN 1077-8926, Vol. 17, no 1, p. N19-Article in journal (Refereed)
  • 16.
    Eriksson, Henrik
    et al.
    KTH.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Linusson, Svante
    KTH.
    Wästlund, Johan
    Linköpings universitet.
    Dense packing of patterns in a permutation2007In: Annals of Combinatorics, ISSN 0218-0006, E-ISSN 0219-3094, Vol. 11, no 3/4, p. 459-470Article in journal (Refereed)
  • 17.
    Eriksson, Henrik
    et al.
    KTH, Sweden.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Sjöstrand, Jonas
    KTH, Sweden.
    Exact expectations for random graphs and assignments2003In: Combinatorics, probability & computing, ISSN 0963-5483, E-ISSN 1469-2163, Vol. 12, p. 401-412Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    For a random graph on n vertices where the edges appear with individual rates, we give exact formulas for the expected time at which the number of components has gone down to k and the expected length of the corresponding minimal spanning forest.

    For a random bipartite graph we give a formula for the expected time at which a k-assignment appears. This result has a bearing on the random assignment problem.

  • 18.
    Eriksson, Henrik
    et al.
    KTH, Sweden.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    KTH, Sweden.
    Sjöstrand, Jonas
    KTH, Sweden.
    Expected inversion number after k adjacent transpositions2000In: Formal Power Series and Algebraic Combinatorics / [ed] D. Krob, A.A. Mikhalev; A.V. Mikhalev, 2000, p. 677-685Conference paper (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    We give expressions for the expected number of inversions after t random adjacent transpositions have been performed on the identity permutation in Sn+1 The problem is a simplification of a problem motivated by genome evolution. For a fixed t and for all n greater than or equal to t, the expected number of inversions after t random adjacent transpositions is

    E-nt = t - 2/n ((t)(2)) + Sigma(r=2)(t) (-1)(r)/n(r) [2(r)C(r)((t)(r+1)) + 4d(r) ((t)(r))]

    where d(2) = 0, d(3) = 1, d(4) = 9, d(5) = 69,... is a certain integer sequence. An important part of the our method is the use of a heat. conduction analogy of the random walks, which guarantees certain properties of the solution.

  • 19.
    Eriksson, Henrik
    et al.
    KTH, Sweden.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Sjöstrand, Jonas
    KTH, Sweden.
    Note on the lamp lighting problem2001In: Advances in Applied Mathematics, ISSN 0196-8858, E-ISSN 1090-2074, Vol. 27, no 2-3, p. 357-366Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    We answer some questions concerning the so-called sigma -game of Sutner [Linear cellular automata and the Garden of Eden, Math. Intelligencer 11 (1989), 49-53]. It is played on a graph where each vertex has a lamp, the light of which is toggled by pressing any vertex with an edge directed to the lamp. For example, we show that every configuration of lamps can be lit if and only if the number of complete matchings in the graph is odd. In the special case of an orthogonal grid one gets a criterion for whether the number of monomer-dimer tilings of an m x n grid is odd or even. 

  • 20.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    A Note on the Exact Expected Length of the kth Part of a Random Partition2010In: Integers, ISSN 1867-0652, Vol. 10, p. 309-311Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Kessler and Livingstone proved an asymptotic formula for the expected length of the largest part of a partition drawn uniformly at random. As a first step they gave an exact formula expressed as a weighted sum of Euler's partition function. Here we give a short bijective proof of a generalization of this exact formula to the expected length of the kth part.

  • 21.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Autism-spectrum traits predict humor styles in the general population2013In: Humor: An International Journal of Humor Research, ISSN 0933-1719, E-ISSN 1613-3722, Vol. 26, no 3, p. 461-475Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Previous research shows that individuals with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism tend to have impaired processing of humor and laugh at things that are not commonly found funny. Here the relationship between humor styles and the broader autism phenotype was investigated in a sample of the general population. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) and the humor styles questionnaire (HSQ) were administered to six hundred US participants recruited through an Internet-based service. On the whole, high scores on AQ were negatively related to positive humor styles and unrelated to negative humor styles. However, AQ subscales representing different autism-spectrum traits exhibited different patterns. In particular, the factor "poor mind-reading" was associated with higher scores on negative humor styles and the factor "attention to detail" was associated with higher scores on all humor styles, suggesting a more nuanced picture of the relationship between autism-spectrum traits and humor.

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 22.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Comment on “The Evolution of Cultural Complexity: Not by the Treadmill Alone” by Andersson & Read2016In: Current Anthropology, ISSN 0011-3204, E-ISSN 1537-5382, Vol. 57, p. 275-276Article in journal (Other academic)
    Abstract [en]

    At the end of their thoughtful target article, Andersson and Read conclude that formal models of cultural evolution are “useful but must be kept in perspective.” As a mathematician with a great interest in social science, I have some experience of working with such models. Based on this experience, I very much agree with the “but” part of the above conclusion. I see a clear tendency in the cultural evolution literature to put too much trust in the value of formal models.

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 23.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Ethnographic stories of cooperation and algebraic stories of evolution2007In: Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, ISSN 0737-4828, Vol. 5, no 1-4, p. 245-250Article, book review (Other (popular science, discussion, etc.))
  • 24.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication. Centre for Cultural Evolution, Stockholm University, Sweden.
    Gender Differences in the Interest in Mathematics Schoolwork Across 50 Countries2020In: Frontiers in Psychology, E-ISSN 1664-1078, Vol. 11, article id 578092Article in journal (Refereed)
  • 25.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Humaniora och matematik – visst går de ihop2006In: Tvärsnitt, ISSN 0348-7997, no 3, p. 12-15Article in journal (Other (popular science, discussion, etc.))
    Abstract [en]

    Matematik är inte bara torra siffror. Det är resonemang och teori som är ämnets kärna. Matematiken erbjuder metoder som gör det svåra lättare att förstå. Därför behöver även humanister få sig lite matte till livs, menar matematikprofessorn Kimmo Eriksson.

  • 26.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Matematikens baksida2006In: Folkvett, ISSN 0283-0795, no 4Article in journal (Other (popular science, discussion, etc.))
  • 27.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Repeated learning and cultural evolution2012In: Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning / [ed] Norbert M. Seel, Springer-Verlag New York, 2012, no 20, p. 2824-2825Chapter in book (Refereed)
  • 28.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Statistical and combinatorial aspects of comparative genomics2004In: Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, ISSN 0303-6898, Vol. 31, no 2, p. 203-216Article, review/survey (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    This document presents a survey of the statistical and combinatorial aspects of four areas of comparative genomics: gene order based measures of evolutionary distances between species, construction of phylogenetic trees, detection of horizontal transfer of genes, and detection of ancient whole genome duplications.

  • 29.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    The Accuracy of Mathematical Models of Justice Evaluations2012In: The Journal of mathematical sociology, ISSN 0022-250X, E-ISSN 1545-5874, Vol. 36, no 2, p. 125-135Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Jasso (1978) proposed a universal law of justice evaluations describing a logarithmic relationship between the perceived injustice of a reward and the ratio between this reward and the just reward. In applications this model is treated as if it were exact, whereas analogous models in psychophysics have empirically established degrees of uncertainty. In this article I make the first assessment of the magnitude of error in the logarithmic model of justice evaluations, using published data and a novel experiment. For the standard application of the model, where just rewards are inferred from justice evaluations, I find that the inherent inaccuracy leads to errors of about 15% on average. I also compared the logarithmic model to 2 nonlogarithmic models. Almost 20% of my respondents made justice evaluations that were more consistent with one of the latter models, suggesting that no single model is really universal.

  • 30.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    The nonsense math effect2012In: Judgment and Decision Making, E-ISSN 1930-2975, Vol. 7, no 6, p. 746-749Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Mathematics is a fundamental tool of research. Although potentially applicable in every discipline, the amount of training in mathematics that students typically receive varies greatly between different disciplines. In those disciplines where most researchers do not master mathematics, the use of mathematics may be held in too much awe. To demonstrate this I conducted an online experiment with 200 participants, all of which had experience of reading research reports anda postgraduate degree (in any subject). Participants were presented with the abstracts from two published papers (one inevolutionary anthropology and one in sociology). Based on these abstracts, participants were asked to judge the quality of the research. Either one or the other of the two abstracts was manipulated through the inclusion of an extra sentence taken from a completely unrelated paper and presenting an equation that made no sense in the context. The abstract that included the meaningless mathematics tended to be judged of higher quality. However, this "nonsense math effect" was not found among participants with degrees in mathematics, science, technology or medicine.

  • 31. Eriksson, Kimmo
    Vad anser MBA-studenter om lön och kön?2005In: Ekonomisk Debatt, ISSN 0345-2646, no 2, p. 49-52Article in journal (Other academic)
  • 32.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Andersson, P. A.
    Linköping University, Sweden.
    Strimling, P.
    Stockholm University, Sweden; Linköping University, Sweden.
    Moderators of the disapproval of peer punishment2016In: Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, ISSN 1368-4302, E-ISSN 1461-7188, Vol. 19, no 2, p. 152-168Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Recent studies have found disapproval of peer punishment of norm violations. This seems puzzling, given the potential benefits peer punishers contribute to the group. We suggest part of the answer is that peer punishers tend to come across as aggressive and as such may be viewed as more problematic than beneficial to have around. We used simple computer animations of geometric shapes to enact 15 precise variations of social sanctions against a norm violator. More than 1,800 subjects were recruited to watch an animation and judge the behavior and character of the animated agents. They also completed a trait aggression measure. Across the variations peer punishment was typically disapproved of, especially when severe or openly aggressive, and especially by subjects low on trait aggression. We conclude that there seems to be a social norm against peer punishment and that dislike of aggressiveness seems to be part of the reason why.

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 33.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Andersson, P. A.
    Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden.
    Strimling, P.
    Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden.
    When is it appropriate to reprimand a norm violation?: The roles of anger, behavioral consequences, violation severity, and social distance2017In: Judgment and Decision Making, E-ISSN 1930-2975, Vol. 12, no 4, p. 396-407Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Experiments on economic games typically fail to find positive reputational effects of using peer punishment of selfish behavior in social dilemmas. Theorists had expected positive reputational effects because of the potentially beneficial consequences that punishment may have on norm violators’ behavior. Going beyond the game-theoretic paradigm, we used vignettes to study how various social factors influence approval ratings of a peer who reprimands a violator of a group-beneficial norm. We found that ratings declined when punishers showed anger, and this effect was mediated by perceived aggressiveness. Thus the same emotions that motivate peer punishers may make them come across as aggressive, to the detriment of their reputation. However, the negative effect of showing anger disappeared when the norm violation was sufficiently severe. Ratings of punishers were also influenced by social distance, such that it is less appropriate for a stranger than a friend to reprimand a violator. In sum, peer punisher ratings were very high for a friend reprimanding a severe norm violation, but particularly poor for a stranger showing anger at a mild norm violation. We found no effect on ratings of whether the reprimand had the beneficial consequence of changing the violator’s behavior. Our findings provide insight into how peer punishers can avoid negative reputational effects. They also point to the importance of going beyond economic games when studying peer punishment. 

  • 34.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Bailey, Drew
    Geary, David
    The grammar of approximating number pairs2010In: Memory & Cognition, ISSN 0090-502X, E-ISSN 1532-5946, Vol. 38, no 3, p. 333-343Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    In the present article, we studied approximating pairs of numbers (a, b) that were used to estimate quantity in a single phrase ("two, three years ago"). Pollmann and Jansen (1996) found that only a few of the many possible pairs are actually used, suggesting an interaction between the ways in which people estimate quantity and their use of quantitative phrases in colloquial speech. They proposed a set of rules that describe which approximating pairs are used in Dutch phrases. We revisited this issue in an analysis of Swedish and American language corpora and in a series of three experiments in which Swedish and American adults rated the acceptability of various approximating pairs and created approximating pairs of their own in response to various estimation tasks. We found evidence for Pollmann and Jansen's rules in both Swedish and English phrases, but we also identified additional rules and substantial individual and cross-language variation. We will discuss implications for the origin of this loose "grammar" of approximating pairs.

  • 35.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Berglund, Lasse
    Jonsson, Mikael
    Gavel, Hillevi
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Tal och Rum: S kurs B2008Book (Other (popular science, discussion, etc.))
  • 36.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Centre for Cultural Evolution, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden..
    Björnstjerna, Marie
    Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden..
    Vartanova, Irina
    Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden..
    The Relation Between Gender Egalitarian Values and Gender Differences in Academic Achievement.2020In: Frontiers in Psychology, E-ISSN 1664-1078, Vol. 11, article id 236Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Gender differences in achievement exhibit variation between domains and between countries. Much prior research has examined whether this variation could be due to variation in gender equality in opportunities, with mixed results. Here we focus instead on the role of a society's values about gender equality, which may have a more pervasive influence. We pooled all available country measures on adolescent boys' and girls' academic achievement between 2000 and 2015 from the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) and Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) assessments of math, science, and reading. We then analyzed the relation between gender differences and country levels of gender egalitarian values, controlling for country levels of living standards and indicators of gender equality in opportunities. Gender egalitarian values came out as the most important predictor. Specifically, more gender egalitarian values were associated with improved performance of boys relative to girls in the same countries. This pattern held in reading, where boys globally perform substantially worse than girls, as well as in math and science where gender differences in performance are small and may favor either boys or girls. Our findings suggest a previously underappreciated role of cultural values in moderating gender gaps in academic achievement.

  • 37.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Coultas, J. C.
    Stockholm University, Sweden.
    De Barra, M.
    Stockholm University, Sweden.
    Cross-Cultural Differences in Emotional Selection on Transmission of Information2016In: Journal of Cognition and Culture, ISSN 1567-7095, E-ISSN 1568-5373, Vol. 16, no 1-2, p. 122-143Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Research on cultural transmission among Americans has established a bias for transmitting stories that have disgusting elements (such as exposure to rats and maggots). Conceived of as a cultural evolutionary force, this phenomenon is one type of emotional selection. In a series of online studies with Americans and Indians we investigate whether there are cultural differences in emotional selection, such that the transmission process favours different kinds of content in different countries. The first study found a bias for disgusting content (rats and maggots) among Americans but not among Indians. Four subsequent studies focused on how country interacts with kind of emotional content (disgusting vs. happy surprises and good news) in reactions to transmission of stories or information. Whereas Indian participants, compared to Americans, tended to be less interested in, and excited by, transmission of stories and news involving common disgust-elicitors (like rats), the opposite pattern held for transmission of happy surprises and good news (e.g., the opening of a new public facility). We discuss various possible explanations and implications. 

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 38.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Coultas, Julie
    University of Sussex.
    Are people really conformist-biased?: An empirical test and a new mathematical model2009In: Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, ISSN 0737-4828, Vol. 7, no 1, p. 5-21Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    According to an influential theory in cultural evolution, within-group similarity of culture is explained by a human conformist-bias, which is a hypothesized evolved predisposition to preferentially follow a member of the majority when acquiring ideas and behaviours. However, this notion has little support from social psychological research. In fact, a major theory in social psychology (Latan and Wolf (1981) argues for what is in effect a nonconformist-bias: by analogy to standard psychophysics they predict minority sources of influence to have relatively greater impact than majority sources. Here we present a new mathematical model and an experiment on social influence, both specifically designed to test these competing predictions. The results are in line with nonconformism. Finally, we discuss within-group similarity and suggest that it is not a general phenomenon but must be studied trait by trait.

  • 39.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Coultas, Julie
    Department of Psychology, University of Sussex.
    Theory of conformist social learning2012In: Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning / [ed] Norbert M Seel, Springer-Verlag New York, 2012, no 20, p. 3314-3316Chapter in book (Refereed)
  • 40.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Coultas, Julie C.
    Stockholm University.
    Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends2014In: Journal of Cognition and Culture, ISSN 1567-7095, E-ISSN 1568-5373, Vol. 14, no 1-2, p. 1-26Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    In one conception of cultural evolution, the evolutionary success of cultural units that are transmitted from individual to individual is determined by forces of cultural selection. Here we argue that it is helpful to distinguish between several distinct phases of the transmission process in which cultural selection can operate, such as a choose-toreceive phase, an encode-and-retrieve phase, and a choose-to-transmit phase. Here we focus on emotional selection in cultural transmission of urban legends, which has previously been shown to operate in the choose-to-transmit phase. In a series of experiments we studied serial transmission of stories based on urban legends manipulated to be either high or low on disgusting content. Results supported emotional selection operating in all three phases of cultural transmission. Thus, the prevalence of disgusting urban legends in North America may be explained by emotional selection through a multitude of pathways. 

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 41.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication.
    Coultas, Julie C.
    Univ Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom.
    The advantage of multiple cultural parents in the cultural transmission of stories2012In: Evolution and human behavior, ISSN 1090-5138, E-ISSN 1879-0607, Vol. 33, no 4, p. 251-259Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Recent mathematical modeling of repeated cultural transmission has shown that the rate at which culture is lost (due to imperfect transmission) will crucially depend on whether individuals receive transmissions from many cultural parents or only from one. However, the modeling assumptions leading up to this conclusion have so far not been empirically assessed. Here we do this for the special case of transmission chains where each individual either receives the same story twice from one cultural parent (and retransmits twice to a cultural child) or receives possibly different versions of the story from two cultural parents (and then retransmits to two cultural children). For this case, we first developed a more general mathematical model of cultural retention that takes into account the possibility of dependence of error rates between transmissions. In this model, under quite plausible assumptions, chains with two cultural parents will have superior retention of culture. This prediction was then tested in two experiments using both written and oral modes of transmission. In both cases, superior retention of culture was found in chains with two cultural parents. Estimation of model parameters indicated that error rates were not identical and independent between transmissions; instead, a primacy effect was suggested, such that the first transmission tends to have higher fidelity than the second transmission.

  • 42.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Center for the Study of Cultural Evolution, Stockholm University, Sweden.
    Cownden, Daniel
    Center for the Study of Cultural Evolution, Stockholm University, Sweden.
    Ehn, Micael
    Center for the Study of Cultural Evolution, Stockholm University, Sweden.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Center for the Study of Cultural Evolution, Stockholm University, Sweden.
    ‘Altruistic’ and ‘antisocial ’ punishers are one and the same2014In: Review of Behavioral Economics, ISSN 2326-6198, E-ISSN 2326-6201, Vol. 1, no 3, p. 1-13Article in journal (Refereed)
    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 43.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm Univ, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Cownden, Daniel
    Stockholm Univ, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Stockholm Univ, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Social learning may lead to population level conformity without individual level frequency bias2017In: Scientific Reports, E-ISSN 2045-2322, Vol. 7, article id 17341Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    A requirement of culture, whether animal or human, is some degree of conformity of behavior within populations. Researchers of gene-culture coevolution have suggested that population level conformity may result from frequency-biased social learning: individuals sampling multiple role models and preferentially adopting the majority behavior in the sample. When learning from a single role model, frequency-bias is not possible. We show why a population-level trend, either conformist or anticonformist, may nonetheless be almost inevitable in a population of individuals that learn through social enhancement, that is, using observations of others' behavior to update their own probability of using a behavior in the future. The exact specification of individuals' updating rule determines the direction of the trend. These results offer a new interpretation of previous findings from simulations of social enhancement in combination with reinforcement learning, and demonstrate how results of dynamical models may strongly depend on seemingly innocuous choices of model specifications, and how important it is to obtain empirical data on which to base such choices.

  • 44.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, 101 31, Sweden.
    Dickins, T. E.
    Middlesex University, The Burroughs, London, Hendon, United Kingdom.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Institute for Futures Studies, Box 591, 101 31 Stockholm, Sweden.
    Hygiene Norms Across 56 Nations are Predicted by Self-Control Values and Disease Threat2021In: Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, ISSN 2666-6227, Vol. 2, article id 100013Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Three major theories could potentially explain why hygiene norms vary across societies: tightness-looseness theory, disease threat theory, and theory of a civilizing process driven by how self-control is valued. We test these theories using data from a study of 56 countries across the globe, in which almost 20,000 participants reported their norms about spitting in six different contexts, hand washing in six different contexts, and tooth brushing. Participants also reported the perceived tightness of their society, whether they perceived diseases as a threat to their society, and their valuation of self-control. In support of the civilizing process, most of the norms in our study (including most hand washing norms and most spitting norms) were stricter in countries where self-control is valued more highly. A few norms did not follow this main pattern and these norms were instead stricter in countries where disease was perceived as a greater threat. Thus, while the theory of a civilizing process received the strongest support, our data indicate that some combination with the disease threat theory may be required to fully explain country-variation in hygiene norms.

  • 45.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Dickins, Thomas E.
    Department of Psychology, School of Science & Technology, Middlesex University, Hendon, London, United Kingdom.
    Strimling, Pontus
    Institute for Futures Studies, Stockholm, Sweden.
    Global sex differences in hygiene norms and their relation to sex equality2022In: PLOS Global Public Health, E-ISSN 2767-3375, Vol. 2, no 6, article id e0000591Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Strict norms about hygiene may sometimes have health benefits but may also be a burden. Based on research in the United States, it has been suggested that women traditionally shoulder responsibility for hygiene standards and therefore tend to have stricter views on hygiene. However, there is little systematic research on sex differences in hygiene norms at the global scale. We set up two hypotheses: (1) Stricter hygiene norms among women than among men is a global phenomenon. (2) The size of this sex difference varies across nations with the level of sex equality. We examine these hypotheses using data from a recent international survey (N = 17,632). Participants in 56 countries were asked for their views of where it is not appropriate for people to spit and in which situations people should wash their hands. As a measure of sex equality, we use an existing country-level measure of attitudes to equality between the sexes, available for 49 nations in the study. Stricter hygiene norms among women than among men are observed almost everywhere, but there are a few exceptions (most notably Nigeria and Saudi Arabia). The size of the sex difference in hygiene norms varies strongly with the level of sex equality, but in a non-linear way. The sex difference is most pronounced in moderately egalitarian countries with the highest recorded difference being in Chile. In more egalitarian parts of the world, more sex equality is associated with a smaller sex difference in hygiene norms. In the less egalitarian parts of the world, the opposite relation holds. We offer an interpretation in terms of what different levels of sex equality mean for the content of sex roles.

  • 46.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Eliasson, J.
    Stockholm Transport Administration, Stockholm, Sweden.
    The Chicken Braess Paradox2019In: Mathematics Magazine, ISSN 0025-570X, E-ISSN 1930-0980, Vol. 92, no 3, p. 213-221Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Summary.: The Braess Paradox is the counterintuitive fact that creation of a shortcut may make travel slower. As each driver seeks to minimize his/her travel time, the shortcut may become so popular that it causes congestion elsewhere in the road network, thereby increasing the travel time for everyone. We extend the paradox by considering a shortcut that is a single-lane but two-way street. The conflict about which drivers get to use the single-lane shortcut is an example of a game theoretic situation known as Chicken, which merges with the Braess Paradox into the novel Chicken Braess Paradox: meeting traffic may make travel quicker. 

  • 47.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics.
    Enquist, Magnus
    Stockholm University.
    Ghirlanda, Stefano
    Stockholm University.
    Critical points in current theory of conformist social learning2007In: Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, ISSN 0737-4828, Vol. 5, no 1, p. 67-87Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Existing mathematical models suggest that gene-culture coevolution favours a conformist bias in social learning, that is, a psychological mechanism to preferentially acquire the most common cultural variants. Here we show that this conclusion relies on specific assumptions that seem unrealistic, such as that all cultural variants are known to every individual. We present two models that remove these assumptions, showing that: 1) the rate of cultural evolution and the adaptive value of culture are higher in a population in which individuals pick cultural variants at random ( Random strategy) rather than picking the most common one ( Conform strategy); 2) in genetic evolution the Random strategy out-competes the Conform strategy, unless cultural evolution is very slow, in which caseConform and Random usually coexist; 3) the individuals’ ability to evaluate cultural variants is a more important determinant of the adaptive value of culture than frequency-based choice strategies. We also review existing empirical literature and game-theoretic arguments for conformity, finding neither strong empirical evidence nor a strong theoretical expectation for a general conformist bias. Our own vignette study of social learning shows that people may indeed use different social learning strategies depending on context.

  • 48.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm University, Sweden.
    Funcke, Alexander
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics. Stockholm University, Sweden; Harvard University, United States .
    A Below-Average Effect with Respect to American Political Stereotypes on Warmth and Competence2015In: Political Psychology, ISSN 0162-895X, E-ISSN 1467-9221, Vol. 36, no 3, p. 341-350Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    The "above-average effect" is the phenomenon that people tend to judge themselves above average on desirable traits. Based on social identity theory, we propose that a "below-average effect" may arise when individuals rate themselves and the average ingroup member on traits stereotypically associated with the ingroup. In two studies, Republican and Democrat participants rated themselves and the average political ingroup member on possession of desirable traits related to warmth and competence. Current political stereotypes in America associate the former dimension with Democrats and the latter with Republicans. Consistent with our hypothesis, the above-average effect was moderated by political group and dimension in interaction. In particular, Democrats rated themselves below the average Democrat on warmth and Republicans rated themselves below the average Republican on competence. 

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 49.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Funcke, Alexander
    Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Humble Self-Enhancement: Religiosity and the Better-Than-Average Effect2014In: Social Psychology and Personality Science, ISSN 1948-5506, E-ISSN 1948-5514, Vol. 5, no 1, p. 76-83Article in journal (Refereed)
    Abstract [en]

    Prior research has linked religiosity to certain forms of self-enhancement. We extend this literature by three studies linking religiosity to the well-established better-than-average effect (BAE). First, a reanalysis of self-judgments of desirable characteristics in 15 nations showed that the BAE was stronger in more religious countries, even taking into account gross domestic product, interdependence, and economic inequality. Second, in two online surveys totaling 1,000 Americans, the BAE was stronger among more religious individuals. Several observations indicated that this relation was due to individuals self-stereotyping with respect to their religious in-groups. In particular, the relation was restricted to characteristics on the warmth dimension, consistent with the religious stereotype, and the average religious in-group member tended to be judged even more favorably than self. The latter phenomenon, which we term humble self-enhancement, is consistent with other studies linking stronger religiosity to greater favoritism of the religious in-group and greater derogation of religious out-groups. 

    Download full text (pdf)
    fulltext
  • 50.
    Eriksson, Kimmo
    et al.
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics. Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Gavel, Hillevi
    Mälardalen University, Department of Mathematics and Physics. Mälardalen University, School of Education, Culture and Communication, Educational Sciences and Mathematics.
    Diskret matematik: Fördjupning2003Book (Other (popular science, discussion, etc.))
123 1 - 50 of 149
CiteExportLink to result list
Permanent link
Cite
Citation style
  • apa
  • ieee
  • modern-language-association-8th-edition
  • vancouver
  • Other style
More styles
Language
  • de-DE
  • en-GB
  • en-US
  • fi-FI
  • nn-NO
  • nn-NB
  • sv-SE
  • Other locale
More languages
Output format
  • html
  • text
  • asciidoc
  • rtf